: Marian Klamer
: A Grammar of Teiwa Grammar of Teiwa
: De Gruyter Mouton
: 9783110226072
: Mouton Grammar Library [MGL]ISSN
: 1
: CHF 262.10
:
: Allgemeine und Vergleichende Sprachwissenschaft
: English
: 558
: Wasserzeichen/DRM
: PC/MAC/eReader/Tablet
: PDF
< >Teiwa is a non-Austronesian ('Papuan') language spoken on the island of Pantar, in eastern Indonesia. It has approximately 4,000 speakers and is highly endangered. The genetic relationship between the Alor-Pantar languages and other Papuan languages remains controversial. Located some 1,000 km from their putative Papuan neighbors on the New Guinea mainland, they are the most distant westerly Papuan outliers. This volume presents a grammatical description of one of these 'outlier' languages. The grammar is based on primary field data, collected by the author in 2003-2007. A selection of glossed and translated Teiwa texts of various genres and word lists (Teiwa-English / English-Teiwa) are included.


< TRONG>Marian Klamer, Leiden University, The Netherlands.

Chapter 11 Information structure (p. 397-398)

11.0. Introduction

This chapter presents a sketch of some aspects of Teiwa‘information packaging’ (Chafe 1976) or‘information structure’ (Lambrecht 1994). The information structure of a sentence is the formal expression of the pragmatic structuring of a proposition in a discourse (Lambrecht 1994:5). That is, in discourse, speakers can assume that there is information about already mentioned events and participants common to both them and their addressees, but other information which is not. The structuring of sentences along these parameters is called‘information structure’ (cf. Foley 2007: 403).

Crosslinguistically, information structure can be formally manifested in different ways: in aspects of prosodic structure such as focus stress; in special grammatical markers for topic or focus; in the form of nominal constituents and the ordering of such constituents in the sentence; in the form of complex grammatical constructions such as passives or antipassives; and in certain choices between related lexical items. A selection of these formal manifestations of information structure are found in Teiwa and discussed in this chapter. Here, I focus on those aspects of Teiwa information structure that I found most striking when I was examining and analysing the texts in my corpus. Note that the formal domain of information structure is the clause or sentence, so that the organisation of Teiwa discourse will not be discussed here.

The chapter is structured as follows. In section 11.1, I address the question how discourse participants are introduced in Teiwa texts. In section 11.2, I describe how constituents get contrastive or identificational focus by placing them in alternative positions in the clause, and by using long rather than short pronouns. In section 11.3, I describe the formal and functional characteristics of focus expressions marked with the focus marker la. La follows the phrase it governs and typically marks new information focus. While focused constituents are new information, just being introduced into the discourse, topics are closely related with given or old information. Section 11.4 describes the topic marker ta, which functions to encode topic discontinuity by announcing a new topic or reintroducing one that was introduced previously. In section 11.5, I briefly indicate the discourse function of two demonstratives that are frequently used in texts: i, which marks a forthcoming topic, and waal‘the one mentioned’, which refers to participants that have been introduced before.

11.1. The introduction of discourse participants

New discourse participants are nominal constituents (NPs) that occur with various modifiers, including nuk‘one’, which marks indefinite NPs (Ch. 5, section 5.5.1), and u‘Distal’, which marks definite NPs (Ch. 3, section 3.6.1). New discourse participants are introduced into the discourse by their own separate predicate: a verb of motion, posture, a deictic verb, or the existential verb pas‘be exactly’. After they have been introduced, the now given/known participants are referred to by short pronouns, or are not expressed at all, which results in zero anaphora across clauses.

Acknowledgements6
Table of contents10
Abbreviations18
Chapter 1. Introduction20
Chapter 2. Phonology56
Chapter 3. Word classes86
Chapter 4. Grammatical relations182
Chapter 5. The Noun Phrase206
Chapter 6. Non-verbal clauses240
Chapter 7. Verbal clauses: The marking of Reality status, Modality and Aspect264
Chapter 8. Negative, interrogative, and imperative clauses292
Chapter 9. Serial verb constructions322
Chapter 10. Clause combinations380
Chapter 11. Information structure416
Appendix I. Texts444
Appendix II. Word lists470
Notes530
References544
Index554